Not always a matter of context: direct effects of red on arousal but context-dependent moderations on valence (2024)

  • Journal List
  • PeerJ
  • PMC5045874

As a library, NLM provides access to scientific literature. Inclusion in an NLM database does not imply endorsem*nt of, or agreement with, the contents by NLM or the National Institutes of Health.
Learn more: PMC Disclaimer | PMC Copyright Notice

Not always a matter of context: direct effects of red on arousal but context-dependent moderations on valence (1)

Link to Publisher's site

PeerJ. 2016; 4: e2515.

Published online 2016 Sep 27. doi:10.7717/peerj.2515

PMCID: PMC5045874

PMID: 27703858

Vanessa L. BuechnerNot always a matter of context: direct effects of red on arousal but context-dependent moderations on valence (2) and Markus A. Maier

Academic Editor: Stefano Ferraina

Author information Article notes Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

Associated Data

Data Availability Statement

Abstract

The arousal theory of color proposes that red is associated with arousal. Research on the color-in-context theory, in turn, states that the context in which red is perceived influences its valence-related meaning and behavioral responses to it. This study faces and integrates these theories by examining the influence of red on both arousal and valence perceptions of test-relevant and neutral stimuli, rendering a color 2 (red vs. blue) × context 2 (test vs. neutral) between-subjects design. Participants rated different pictures regarding their arousal and valence component, respectively. In line with the assumptions of both theories, red increased arousal perceptions of stimuli irrespective of their valence but a context × color interaction was found for valence perceptions: for participants viewing test-relevant pictures, red increased their perceptions of negativity compared to neutral pictures. The present study shows that both theories are actually compatible when differentiating the arousal and valence component.

Keywords: Color, Red, Arousal, Valence, Context

Introduction

Research on the meaning and effects of colors has been of interest since the 1940’s. Thereby, two different theories primarily focusing on effects of the color red met approval in the last years—the arousal theory of color and the color-in-context theory. As their names imply, the arousal theory states that the color red per se is arousing, whereas the color-in-context theory states that context in which this color is perceived influences its valence-related meaning and behavioral responses to it. Plenty of research has been done on both theories and will be briefly described in the following paragraph.

In 1942 Goldstein stated that color perception produces physiological reactions in the body that are manifest in emotions, cognitions, and behavior (Goldstein, 1942). Based on this idea, later color researchers posited that longer wavelength colors, such as red, are experienced as arousing and stimulating, whereas shorter wavelength colors, such as green and blue, are relaxing (e.g., Crowley, 1993). Nakshian (1964) provided support for the notion that red is physiologically exciting or arousing, as red, in comparison to green, leads to greater hand tremor and faster speed of movement in a motor inhibition task. Similarly, Gerard (1958) showed that the perception of red light leads to an increase of blood pressure, respiratory movements, and frequency of eye blink, whereas blue light leads to a decrease of these parameters. Interestingly, Wilson (1966) showed that red, in comparison to green, is not only more arousing from an implicit physiological (i.e., high skin conductance level and response) but also from a more explicit cognitive (i.e., ratings) perspective. Using Likert scales research could show that the color red is associated with terms such as ‘exciting’ and ‘stimulating’ (Wexner, 1954) as well as ‘warmth’ and ‘less calmness’ ().

Taken together, previous research using both explicit and implicit measurement tools provided evidence that the color red, as a typical long-wavelength color, is correlated with high arousal whereas short-wavelength colors reduce the individual’s arousal level.

The second theory, the color-in-context theory () focuses on the valence dimension of colors and, by drawing on biology and social learning, claims that the context in which color is perceived influences its affective meaning and responses to it. Context is defined as both the physical structure as well as the psychological situation (; ) within which color is embedded. These circ*mstances frame a color and determine its meaning. More precisely, with regard to the color red in achievement contexts this color becomes associated with both ‘danger’ and ‘caution’ () and therefore activates avoidance motivation (Elliot et al., 2009; ). In romantic contexts, however, red becomes associated with ‘sex’ and ‘romance’ and consequently triggers approach motivation (Elliot et al., 2010; ; ; Meier et al., 2012). Thus, depending on the situational circ*mstances red seems to take on opposite meanings on the valence dimension (see also Maier et al., 2009).

The original color-in-context theory favored an association approach assuming that red is associated with positive valence and approach tendencies in one context (e.g., dating) and negative valence and avoidance tendencies within other contextual circ*mstances (e.g., achievement or threat to life). However, this model has recently been refined with regard to the exact nature of how the color red interacts with the context. Instead of the activation of different associations in the presence of red through various contexts, red was proposed to exert an attentional bias towards the object that is displaying the color red. More precisely, Buechner and colleagues could show that red actually functions as a signal of importance, which carries the information that a stimulus is worthy of attention. This attentional bias in turn emphasizes a stimulus’ existing motivational message and consequently increases the perceivers’ appropriate -that is valence dependent- response tendencies: increased approach in case of a positive-appetitive stimulus configuration and increased avoidance in case of a negative-aversive one (Buechner et al., 2014; Buechner et al., 2015). This was shown in a study using a head and upper torso photo of a young man wearing a white shirt. Color was manipulated by placing a red or blue circle on the left chest of the target male and the type of facial expression and body posture (i.e., proud and ashamed male; see University of California-Davis Set of Emotion Expressions; ) constituted the context. The results showed that in female mate evaluation, red increases the perceived attractiveness of a proud man (i.e., an appetitive signal of mate value) but tends to decrease the perceived attractiveness of an ashamed man (i.e., an aversive signal of low mate value). To sum up, the updated color-in-context theory claims that red accentuates a stimulus’ affective meaning and subsequently defines or triggers the corresponding valence of this stimulus. Thus, the proposed impact color might exert in the present study is different from previous studies: it slightly intensifies the meaning of the negative test items, but does not change it. Importantly, as predicted, red effects that vary with the context have only been found on the valence dimension of the stimuli used.

In light of both theories, red is expected to increase arousal regardless of contextual variations in which the color is perceived, but red is supposed to trigger the perception of valence along with and therefore dependent on the context-related information being present.

Aims and hypotheses of the present research

This study aims to examine both the arousal and the color-in-context hypothesis by exploring the influence of colored stimuli on arousal and valence perceptions. The study described herein is thus a conceptual replication of previous findings documenting the intensifying effect of the color red on valence perceptions (Buechner et al., 2015) with a highly powered sample in another context, that is achievement context instead of romantic. Further, experimental studies often used picture stimuli to create romantic settings, but never to create achievement contexts. Achievement contexts were mostly induced via instructions or specific performance tasks. Here we fill this gap by using test-relevant and neutral pictures to create an achievement context. That is, a set of test-relevant and neutral pictures will be presented together with a red or blue frame. Red should raise arousal perceptions for both picture types thus confirming the arousal theory but it should increase valence perceptions only for test-relevant pictures confirming the color-in-context theory. 1

Materials & Methods

In line with Simmons, Nelson, and Simonsohn’s (2012) proposal for a 21 word solution of disclosure, we report determination of sample size, all data exclusions, all manipulations, and all measures in the study.

Participants

Before the beginning of the study we performed an a priori power analysis using G*Power 3.1.3 (Faul et al., 2007). Given a color 2 (red vs. blue) × context 2 (test vs. neutral) between-subjects design with four separate experimental groups, the analysis suggested a sample size of 309 participants to provide 80% power at α = 0.05 assuming a small effect size (d = .162). Thus, a total of 310 undergraduate students at German universities participated in the study, 71 in the red test condition, 77 in the red neutral condition, 81 in the blue test condition, and 76 in the blue neutral condition. Five participants were excluded due to color blindness, resulting in a final sample of 305 (74 males, mean age = 22.28 years, SD = 5.52) undergraduate students. This is the only study on color and arousal and valence perceptions that we have conducted.

Design and procedure

Participants were randomly assigned to one of the four experimental conditions, rendering a color 2 (red vs. blue) × context 2 (test vs. neutral) between-subjects design. The experimenters were blind to condition and hypotheses. Participants were instructed to open a folder to view 4 different pictures, around 6.7-in. × 4.5-in. in size and printed with an Epson Stylus Photo R800 color printer on a white heavyweight Epson Matte Paper, 8.2-in. × 11.6-in. in size. We handed four pictures to enable enough stimuli variability and to reduce the likelihood of unknown confounds. More precisely, dependent on the experimental group, they either saw four red-primed test-relevant pictures, 4 blue-primed test-relevant pictures, four red-primed neutral pictures, or four blue-primed neutral pictures. Three neutral pictures were taken from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS; ), which provides an experimental set of 1,169 digitized photographs with normative rating scores (using a 9-point rating scale) on valence and arousal. The neutral pictures were selected based on the normative valence ratings and showed scenarios, such as a theater hall (picture #2037), a newspaper (#7061), or a person reading a book (picture #2377). The forth neutral picture showing a stage was retrieved from the internet and judged as neutral by two dependent experts beforehand. Content-matched negative, test-relevant pictures, retrieved from the internet and judged as negative by two dependent experts beforehand, showed scenarios, such as a lecture hall while students writing an exam, a person being frustrated while working on a paper, a person being frustrated while reading a book, or a lecture hall. Color was manipulated by encircling the pictures with a red or blue frame, 0.3-in. × 0.3-in. in size. A spectrophotometer was used to determine the precise parameters of the colors: red (LCh[55,5; 52,2; 32,0]) and blue (LCh[55,2; 52,0; 274,7]); lightness and chroma were equated across hue. After participants viewed each of the respective four photos (dependent of condition) for five seconds, they were instructed to complete the questionnaire containing the dependent measures of both arousal and valence, demographic items, as well as some control items (i.e., trait and test anxiety) to test for any moderating effects. Someone could assume that high anxious individuals might exhibit a stronger color × context on valence effect. No specific effects on arousal have been hypothesized.

Measures

Valence and arousal

For each of the 4 pictures 1 item was used to assess valence (i.e., “Describe how unpleasant do you think this situation is”) and 1 item to assess arousal (i.e., “Describe how physiologically arousing do you think this situation is”). Participants responded on 1 (not at all) to 9 (extremely) scales for both measures, and the scales were labeled only on point 1 and 9. In between no labels were given as this has been shown to be sufficient and an understandable scale labelling for different variables in previous studies (e.g., Elliot et al., 2010).

Trait anxiety

Based on Spielberger et al.’s (1983) Trait Anxiety Inventory, the German 20-item measure by Laux et al. (1981) was used to assess trait anxiety (e.g., “I worry too much over something that really doesn’t matter”; α = .87). Participants responded on a 1 (almost never) to 4 (almost always) scale.

Test anxiety

Pekrun et al.’s (2004) 12-item Test-Emotions Questionnaire (TEQ) was used to assess test anxiety before exam (e.g., “I worry whether I learned enough”; α = .75) and during exam (e.g., “I feel panicky when writing an exam”; α = .77). Participants responded on 1 (not at all true) to 5 (absolutely true) scales.

Ethics statement

The research reported herein was conducted at the LMU Munich and was approved by the ethics committee of the Department of Psychology, LMU Munich, in accordance with the ethical standards of the American Psychological Association (APA). All participants gave written informed consent and were thoroughly debriefed. The individuals’ consent was obtained after reading the instruction to the experiments. The experimenter asked for the participant’s consent and emphasized that they will receive their credit also if they decided not to participate in this study. Participants were also told that they could stop and leave the experiment at any point of time. Data were stored and analyzed anonymously. This consent procedure has been approved by the ethics committee.

Results

Arousal

A between-subjects analysis of variance (ANOVA) with color and context as between-subjects factors was conducted on arousal. Results revealed no significant two-way interaction on arousal, F = 1.10, p > .250. However, there was a significant main effect for color (F(1, 301) = 4.36, p = .038, ηp2=.01, d = .20) and a significant main effect for context (F(1, 301) = 115.64, p < .001, ηp2=.28, d = 1.23). Participants rated the red pictures (M = 3.83, SD = 1.16) more arousing than the blue ones (M = 3.65, SD = 1.06, see Fig. 1) and the test pictures (M = 4.32, SD = 1.05) more arousing than the neutral ones (M = 3.17, SD = .84).

Open in a separate window

Figure 1

Arousal perceptions of red and blue pictures.

Valence

A further ANOVA with color and context as between-subjects factors was conducted on valence. There was a significant main effect of context (F(1, 301) = 325.48, p < .001, ηp2=.52, d = 2.05). Participants rated the test pictures (M = 4.49, SD = 1.03) more negative than the neutral ones (M = 2.47, SD = .95). In addition, there was a significant main effect for color (F(1, 301) = 5.40, p = .021, ηp2=.02). Participants rated the red pictures (M = 3.57, SD = 1.42) more negative than the blue ones (M = 3.39, SD = 1.41) and this main effect was moderated by a significant two-way interaction, F(1, 301) = 4.12, p = .043, ηp2=.01, d = .16.

To determine the exact nature of this moderation we additionally ran Scheffe post-hoc tests within each context condition. In the test condition, participants rated the pictures more negatively in the red (M = 4.75, SD = .86) than in the blue (M = 4.26, SD = 1.12) condition, p = .026. In the neutral condition, no difference emerged between red and blue pictures, p > .25. Additional post-hoc tests within the color conditions revealed that in the red condition, participants rated the test pictures (M = 4.75, SD = .86) more negative than the neutral ones (M = 2.49, SD = .86), p < .001. In the blue condition, participants rated the test pictures (M = 4.26, SD = 1.12) also more negative than the neutral ones (M = 2.45, SD = 1.05), p < .001, however to a lesser degree (see Fig. 2). 3

Open in a separate window

Valence perceptions of red test-relevant, red neutral, blue test-relevant, and blue neutral pictures.

Supplementary analyses controlling for both covariates, trait and test anxiety, showed that these measures of individual differences did not substantially moderate the interaction effect on valence reported above.

Discussion

The present study examined the influence of red on both arousal and valence perceptions of test-relevant and neutral stimuli. As mentioned before, arousal theorists, like Wexner (1954) and Goldstein (1942), state that the color red per se is arousing. More precisely, they claim that the perception of red is associated with terms such as ‘stimulating’ as well as produces physiological reactions such as faster speed of movement, increased blood pressure, and frequency of eye blink. Research on the color-in-context theory in contrast, could show that context in which the color red is perceived influences its valence-related meaning and behavioral responses to it (e.g., ; Maier et al., 2009).

With regard to the arousal theory, red was expected to raise the present study’s arousal perceptions for both picture types (i.e., test-relevant and neutral ones), as red is supposed to be arousing independently of a given context (see for instance Crowley, 1993; Gerard, 1958; Nakshian, 1964; Wilson, 1966). However, with regard to the color-in-context theory red was expected to raise valence perceptions only for test-relevant pictures, as context is said to be the determining factor. Context dependent moderations of red have consistently been shown for valence related variations. As such, Buechner et al. (2015) could show that red increases the perceivers’ valence dependent response tendencies, that is increased approach in case of a positive-appetitive stimulus configuration and increased avoidance in case of a negative-aversive one. Further, Buechner et al. (2014) could show that such context dependent red effects could be explained by attentional bias towards a specific stimulus. More precisely, red has been shown to accentuate the relevance of a goal-related stimulus and correspondingly intensify the perceivers’ attentional reaction to it.

In line with these predictions the perception of red appears to be arousing, no matter if the stimuli is neutral or negative (i.e., test-relevant), but with regard to valence the perception of red intensifies the negativity only of aversive stimuli. Controlling for the covariates, trait and test anxiety, did not substantially moderate the interaction effect on valence. As such, independently of the trait and test anxiety of subjects, reddened test-relevant pictures were perceived as more negative than blue test-relevant ones as well as red neutral ones.

Previous research has shown that the recognition of facial and bodily expressions is influenced by the context (e.g., De Gelder et al., 2006; ; ). That is, the affective information provided by the surrounding scene in which a face/body appears may influence how it is encoded (). Congruence between the valence of a facial or bodily expression with the valence of a context enhance the emotion perception. In the study presented herein, we used pictures that present congruent target-context information to make sure that the valence is best perceived.

Our results were in line with previous research on both the arousal theory of color (from a cognitive perspective) and the color-in-context theory, and contribute to the psychological literature by facing both theories within one study. However, the present study is limited in some senses that need to be taken into account for further research. As this study focuses on undergraduates only, further research is needed that concentrates on students or adults. Also, the color manipulation done by a frame raises questions regarding the degree to which the findings would generalize to other types of color presentation. A valuable follow up to this study would be a replication that focuses on another age sample and varies the color manipulation.

As mentioned above, an a priori power analysis was performed based on a small effect size (d = .16). We prefer to refrain from post hoc power analyses, as this procedure is known to be fundamentally flawed (). Nevertheless the central effects reported in our paper are indeed very small. Therefore, another preregistered and confirmatory study could help to clarify the robustness of the color effects. We would like to encourage other independent labs to replicate our findings. However, we still think that our results are worth to be reported since many psychological effects have been shown to be very small (30.44% yielding an r of .10 or less; ). In addition, the predefined sample size based on high a priori power allowed us to interpret the results on a statistical level. The question about the practical impact is legitimate to ask but was not the primary goal of this study. Rather, we tried to test different theories of effects of the color red on arousal and valence perceptions.

The present research was designed to differentiate two color theories within one study. In our view we successfully managed this research goal with a highly powerful sample and carefully designed control conditions. We hope that the present results are useful for further research to extend this type of work.

Funding Statement

This research was supported by the German Research Foundation (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (http://www.dfg.de)) Grant # MA 2447/4-1. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Footnotes

1Someone could argue that the research question is redundant. The arousal theory and the color-in-context theory are based on very different levels of processing. Specifically, the arousal theory posits that red deserves its meaning from its wavelength characteristics, while the color-in-context theory focuses on the affective meaning of red on a more cognitive level. From this perspective, it seems logical that the associated arousal does not change with context. However, although theoretically convincing, empirical evidence is missing. Red effects on arousal have never been studied within an experimental context manipulation. Hence, although the main effect of color red on arousal was widely accepted potential moderating influences were never empirically ruled out. (We would like to thank reviewer 1 for this suggestion).

2The effect size of a similar study (Buechner et al, 2015) was small (d = .20). To be more conservative, we assumed a smaller effect size.

3In the demographic section at the end of the experiment, we asked participants to rate how much their perception was affected by the colored frame on a 1 (not at all) to 9 (completely) scale. Subjective ratings showed that most of participants indicated that their judgement was not affected by color (MDN = 2.00, M = 2.33, SD = 1.72). This indicates that the majority of participants in our sample was convinced that color did not affect their behavior in the various tasks they performed. Hence, attributing the observed color effects to an explicit level should be treated with caution. In addition, we also asked our participants to recall the color of the frame. Only 53.77% could correctly remember the color. We speculate that this low recognition rate is another indicator of an incidental, implicit effect of color.

Additional Information and Declarations

Competing Interests

The authors declare there are no competing interests.

Author Contributions

Vanessa L. Buechner conceived and designed the experiments, performed the experiments, analyzed the data, contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools, wrote the paper, prepared figures and/or tables, reviewed drafts of the paper.

Markus A. Maier conceived and designed the experiments, analyzed the data, wrote the paper, reviewed drafts of the paper.

Human Ethics

The following information was supplied relating to ethical approvals (i.e., approving body and any reference numbers):

Ethics committee of the Department of Psychology, LMU Munich.

The research reported herein was conducted at the LMU Munich and was approved by the ethics committee of the Department of Psychology, LMU Munich, in accordance with the ethical standards of the American Psychological Association (APA). All participants gave written informed consent and were thoroughly debriefed. The individuals’ consent was obtained after reading the instruction to the experiments. The experimenter asked for the participant’s consent and emphasized that they will receive their credit also if they decided not to participate in this study. Participants were also told that they could stop and leave the experiment at any point of time. Data were stored and analyzed anonymously. This consent procedure has been approved by the ethics committee.

Data Availability

The following information was supplied regarding data availability:

Buechner V. 2016. Data_Not always a matter of context: Direct effects of red on arousal but context-dependent moderations on valence. Open Science Framework. September 2. Available at osf.io/vzw5r.

References

Bazire & Brézillon (2005) Bazire M, Brézillon P. Understanding context before using it. In: Dey A, Kokinov B, Leake D, Turner R, editors. Context. Springer-Verlag; Berlin: 2005. pp. 29–40. [Google Scholar]

Buechner et al. (2015) Buechner VL, Maier MA, Lichtenfeld S, Elliot AJ. Emotion expression and color: their joint influence on attractiveness and social position. Current Psychology. 2015;34(2):422–433. doi:10.1007/s12144-014-9266-x. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Buechner et al. (2014) Buechner VL, Maier MA, Lichtenfeld S, Schwarz S. Red—take a closer look. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e2515. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0108111. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Crowley (1993) Crowley AE. The two dimensional impact of color on shopping. Marketing Letters. 1993;4:59–69. doi:10.1007/BF00994188. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

De Gelder et al. (2006) De Gelder B, Meeren HK, Righart R, Van den Stock J, Van de Riet WA, Tamietto M. Beyond the face: exploring rapid influences of context on face processing. Progress in Brain Research. 2006;155:37–48. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(06)55003-4. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Elliot & Maier (2012) Elliot AJ, Maier MA. Color-in-context theory. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 2012;45:63–125. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-394286-9.00002-0. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Elliot et al. (2009) Elliot AJ, Maier MA, Binser MJ, Friedman R, Pekrun R. The effect of red on avoidance behavior in achievement contexts. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2009;35:365–375. doi:10.1177/0146167208328330. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Elliot et al. (2010) Elliot AJ, Niesta D, Greitemeyer T, Lichtenfeld S, Gramzow RH, Maier MA, Liu H. Red, rank and romance in women viewing men. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2010;139:399–417. doi:10.1037/a0019689. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Faul et al. (2007) Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social behavioral and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods. 2007;39:175–191. doi:10.3758/BF03193146. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Gerard (1958) Gerard RM. Color and emotional arousal [Abstract from the program of the sixty-sixth annual convention of the American Psychological Association] . Abstract 340American Psychologist. 1958;13 [Google Scholar]

Goldstein (1942) Goldstein K. Some experimental observations concerning the influence of colors on the function of the organism. Occupational Therapy and Rehabilitation. 1942;21:147–151. doi:10.1097/00002060-194206000-00002. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Hoenig & Heisey (2001) Hoenig JM, Heisey DM. The abuse of power: the pervasive fallacy of power calculations for data analysis. The American Statistician. 2001;55(1):19–24. doi:10.1198/000313001300339897. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Kaya & Epps (2004) Kaya N, Epps HH. Relationship between color and emotion: a study of college students. College Student Journal. 2004;38:396–405. [Google Scholar]

Kret & De Gelder (2010) Kret ME, De Gelder B. Social context influences recognition of bodily expressions. Brain Research. 2010;203:169–180. doi:10.1007/s00221-010-2220-8. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Lang, Bradley & Cuthbert (2008) Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN. International affective picture system (IAPS): affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical report A-8 2008

Laux et al. (1981) Laux L, Glanzmann P, Schaffner P, Spielberger CD. Das State-Trait-Angstinventar (Testmappe mit Handanweisung, Fragebogen STAI-G Form X 1 und Fragebogen STAI-G Form X 2) Beltz; Weinheim: 1981. [Google Scholar]

Maier et al. (2009) Maier MA, Barchfeld P, Elliot AJ, Pekrun R. Context-specificity of implicit preferences: the case of human preference for red. Emotion. 2009;9:734–738. doi:10.1037/a0016818. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Maier, Elliot & Lichtenfeld (2008) Maier MA, Elliot AJ, Lichtenfeld S. Mediation of the negative effect of red on intellectual performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2008;34:1530–1540. doi:10.1177/0146167208323104. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Meier et al. (2012) Meier BP, D’Agostino PR, Elliot AJ, Maier MA, Wilkowski BM. Color in context: psychological context moderates the influence of red on approach-and avoidance-motivated behavior. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(7):e40333. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0040333. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Moller, Elliot & Maier (2009) Moller AC, Elliot AJ, Maier MA. Basic hue-meaning associations. Emotion. 2009;9:898–902. doi:10.1037/a0017811. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Nakshian (1964) Nakshian JS. The effects of red and green surroundings on behavior. The Journal of General Psychology. 1964;70:143–161. doi:10.1080/00221309.1964.9920584. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Niesta Kayser, Elliot & Feltman (2010) Niesta Kayser D, Elliot AJ, Feltman R. Red and romantic behavior in men viewing women. European Journal of Social Psychology. 2010;40:901–908. doi:10.1002/ejsp.757. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Pekrun et al. (2004) Pekrun R, Goetz T, Perry RP, Kramer K, Hochstadt M. Beyond test anxiety: development and validation of the Test Emotions Questionnaire (TEQ) Anxiety, Stress, & Coping. 2004;17:287–316. doi:10.1080/10615800412331303847. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Richard, Bond & Stokes-Zoota (2003) Richard FD, Bond CF, Stokes-Zoota JJ. One hundred years of social psychology quantitatively described. Review of General Psychology. 2003;7:331–363. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.7.4.331. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Righart & De Gelder (2006) Righart R, De Gelder B. Context influences early perceptual analysis of faces. An electrophysiological study. Cerebral Cortex. 2006;16:1249–1257. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj066. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Simmons, Nelson & Simonsohn (2012) Simmons JP, Nelson LD, Simonsohn U. A 21 word solution. 2012. Social Science Research Network. http://ssrn.com/abstract=2160588 .

Spielberger et al. (1983) Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene R, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA. Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press; Palo Alto: 1983. [Google Scholar]

Tracy, Robins & Schriber (2009) Tracy JL, Robins RW, Schriber RA. Development of a FACS-verified set of basic and self-conscious emotion expressions. Emotion. 2009;9:554–559. doi:10.1037/a0015766. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Wexner (1954) Wexner LB. The degree to which colors (hues) are associated with mood-tones. Journal of Applied Psychology. 1954;38(6):432–435. doi:10.1037/h0062181. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Wilson (1966) Wilson GD. Arousal properties of red versus green. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 1966;23:947–949. doi:10.2466/pms.1966.23.3.947. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Wright & Rainwater (1962) Wright B, Rainwater L. The meanings of color. The Journal of General Psychology. 1962;67:89–99. doi:10.1080/00221309.1962.9711531. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Zimmermann, Lorenz & Oppermann (2007) Zimmermann A, Lorenz A, Oppermann R. An operational definition of context. In: Dey A, Kokinov B, Leake D, Turner R, editors. Context. Springer-Verlag; Berlin: 2007. pp. 558–571. [Google Scholar]

Articles from PeerJ are provided here courtesy of PeerJ, Inc

Not always a matter of context: direct effects of red on arousal but context-dependent moderations on valence (2024)

FAQs

Does red light cause arousal? ›

However, there are also some uncertainties regarding red light, such as it being more likely to induce arousal and negative emotional experiences (12), and to increase arousal levels and alertness (12–14). Increased excitability, high arousal levels, and anxiety are all detrimental factors to sleep onset.

Why is red such an arousing color? ›

In accordance with these biological occurrences, exposure to the color red causes physiological reactions. “Red has been shown to increase heart rate and blood pressure,” Labrecque says. “Psychologically speaking, red is considered arousing, exciting, and stimulating.”

What psychological effect does the color red have? ›

Red Colour Psychology: The red colour meaning is associated with excitement, passion, danger, energy, and action. You might've noticed that some brands use red for 'order now' buttons or for their packaging as a way to stand out on the shelf.

What color causes arousal? ›

Taken together, previous research using both explicit and implicit measurement tools provided evidence that the color red, as a typical long-wavelength color, is correlated with high arousal whereas short-wavelength colors reduce the individual's arousal level.

What color should arousal be? ›

Arousal fluid is produced in response to sexual stimulation, by glands in and around the vagin* in order to lubricate the vagin* for the possibility of intercourse. The characteristics of arousal fluid are clear, wet, moist, and slippery.

How does red light affect you? ›

Research shows red light therapy may smooth your skin and help with wrinkles. Researchers think it does this by stimulating collagen production, which helps improve skin elasticity. Research shows it also helps improve signs of sun damage. Research has found that red light therapy can be effective for acne treatment.

Why is red so seductive? ›

The Color Red Symbolizes Passion and Desire

But red does not always signal danger and aggression. Perhaps not surprisingly, red also symbolizes passion, love, and desire. These associations could explain why people wearing red are consistently rated as more attractive by the opposite sex.

Why does red grab attention? ›

Red, for example, is typically associated with energy, passion, and excitement. It's not uncommon to see the color associated with brands that want to grab attention. As a bold color that stands out, it's easily noticeable, even from a distance.

What does red symbolize? ›

Since red is the color of blood, it has historically been associated with sacrifice, danger, and courage. Modern surveys in Europe and the United States show red is also the color most commonly associated with heat, activity, passion, sexuality, anger, love, and joy.

What color grabs the most attention? ›

Our eyes have the highest sensitivity towards bright yellow on the visible light spectrum making it a highly visible, attention-grabbing color.

Why does red mean danger? ›

Moller et al. (2009) have shown that people tend to associate red with negative, danger-bearing emotions, since it is the color of fire, blood, anger, and sometimes of poisonous or dangerous animals. Yet, red does not always signal hostility or danger.

What color promotes learning? ›

The color orange encourages critical thinking and memory. Studies have shown that orange has a particularly high effect on circulation and the nervous system. It increases the oxygen supply to the brain, stimulating mental activity and loosening inhibitions.

Why does red cause arousal? ›

The connection between the color red and sexual attraction may be a result of social conditioning. In ancient mythologyand folklore, red is associated with fertility. Similarly, red was used as a symbol of fertility in the rituals of ancient civilizations.

What color makes you harder? ›

Red is a stimulating color that gets your heart pumping and raises your pulse. It is a powerful color that can evoke a strong physical response, even activating the “fight or flight” instinct.

What color is most seductive? ›

However, the color that shows up most frequently on color word association tests as the sexiest shade is a vibrant Lipstick Red. It's a sexual signal that many animals and birds use to attract the opposite sex and, interestingly, humans do as well. They flush and blush, turning red with embarrassment or sexual energy.

Does red increase attraction? ›

The attractiveness-enhancing effect of the color red may reflect its link to perceived health, vitality, and sexual receptivity such as when women's sexual arousal is accompanied by facial blushing [24–26], and when women's facial skin becomes slightly redder during the fertile phase of their menstrual cycle [27, 28].

Does red light make you hot? ›

At low wavelengths, red light doesn't generate heat and penetrates the skin between 1 and 2 millimeters , according to 2013 research. This red light exposure may produce a positive biochemical effect in your cells that strengthens the mitochondria, which is where the cell's energy is created.

Why does red light feel so good? ›

How does red light therapy supposedly work? Red light therapy is thought to work by acting on the “power plant” in your body's cells called mitochondria. With more energy, other cells can do their work more efficiently, doing things like repairing skin, boosting new cell growth and enhancing skin rejuvenation.

Do you feel anything during red light therapy? ›

It likely won't hurt. Red light therapy is noninvasive and is painless for most people. A handheld device may be pressed against the skin at the site of the injury or pain. If you are lying in a full body bed or pod that uses both red and near-infrared light, you may feel warmth from the near-infrared bulbs.

Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Madonna Wisozk

Last Updated:

Views: 6304

Rating: 4.8 / 5 (48 voted)

Reviews: 95% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Madonna Wisozk

Birthday: 2001-02-23

Address: 656 Gerhold Summit, Sidneyberg, FL 78179-2512

Phone: +6742282696652

Job: Customer Banking Liaison

Hobby: Flower arranging, Yo-yoing, Tai chi, Rowing, Macrame, Urban exploration, Knife making

Introduction: My name is Madonna Wisozk, I am a attractive, healthy, thoughtful, faithful, open, vivacious, zany person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.